This Summer Cohort’s QRI Visiting Scholars were tasked with memorizing thirty graphlets and recording their experience over the course of a week. We note that to do so, there usually was a preprocessing step to make the graphlets more easily memorizable, leveraging different types of memory. In particular, the following mnemonic techniques were the most effective:
It was also found that other properties intrinsic to the graphlets, such as their symmetries and their planarity, made memorization easier.
A graphlet, as the name suggests, is a small connected graph. They can certainly be useful for graph-theoretic work, but there are other reasons we may care for them. There are a total of 30 distinct graphlets (with the number of vertices between 2 and 5) up to isomorphism. These objects are simple enough that one can ask a person to memorize all 30 of them in a few days, but complex enough that their memorization will not be a trivial task, and many things can be learned from such an activity. As a refresher on these topics, we suggest Jure Leskovec’s CS224W course, and in particular this lecture which covers graphlets.
Thus, as a phenomenology exercise to kickstart the 2023 QRI Summer Cohort, the three QRI Visiting Scholars (Riccardo, Ethan and Carlos) were tasked with the memorization of the first thirty graphlets (as shown in the picture above) over the course of the first week. They had to record their experiences, as well as any phenomenological observations. In this article we present a few important observations and insights we gather from these experiences.
The set of thirty graphlets, when seen for the first time (and before any patterns are found), seem somewhat arbitrary. It is therefore noteworthy that the three Visiting Scholars, who successfully memorized the graphlets (they were tested and everyone got all the answers correct), required that the objects to be memorized were first ‘processed’ into representations that could be easily memorized by them – the graphlets are divorced enough from our experience as to make it extremely difficult to just memorize them as they are. This ‘preprocessing’ was present in all three attempts, but interestingly, it was not always deliberate, as we will see. Furthermore, different people undertook the preprocessing in different ways:
Something common to all three memorization attempts is that they always involved taking into account, in some way or another, the relations between graphlets: the number of graphlets which were learned ‘in isolation’, without relating them to others, was low. Carlos reported first trying to learn the graphlets through spaced repetition with Anki decks, but he found it to be difficult and time-consuming (although it must be noted that it is still possible to do so) – such an approach would possibly qualify as ‘memorizing the graphlets in isolation’.
There are representations that make use of the fact that we’re good at particular types of memory – such as narrative, or spatial. However, there are other properties that the graphlets as presented have which might also aid with memorization. Some possible properties are the following:
The representations used were predominantly conceptual – however, this doesn’t have to always be the case, as there are many other sorts of representations that so far have been left unexplored: the state-space of consciousness is very rich. Riccardo suggests a dance choreography or sequence of bodily moves as possibilities, which would leverage muscle memory. He also suggested another direction that might be fruitful: embodying the graphlets, and using them as objects of meditation.
Graphlets are a particularly simple mathematical structure: but by virtue of them being mathematical structures, the experience of memorizing them may certainly give us insights into the memorization of, for instance, the 17 two-dimensional wallpaper groups (as mentioned in the Algorithmic Reduction of Psychedelic States), or four-dimensional regular polytopes. And of course, memorization of objects such as these can allow us to properly mathematically describe exotic states of consciousness, such as those experienced during meditation or psychedelic experiences.
Furthermore, the kinds of memorization techniques that seem to work best, as well as their phenomenological qualities, can give us insight into how the mind works and the nature of internal representations:
In other words, the sorts of mnemonic techniques that are the most effective can tell us a great deal about how internal representations are created in the mind.
I want to thank the other two QRI Visiting Scholars, Riccardo Volpato and Ethan Kuntz, for their detailed descriptions of their experience memorizing graphlets, for their insightful feedback, and for our fascinating conversations clarifying some of the ideas found in this article. Furthermore, I want to thank Andrés Gómez Emilsson and Hunter Meyer for their invaluable feedback and comments on the structure and content of this article.
Riccardo (stories):
4 nodes
G3: There was a tree
G4: Which became a small starship
G5: But the small steering wheel didn’t work
G6: So they put it upside down and closed the bottom
G7: Then connected the top and bottom of the steering wheel
G8: And then to the other ends. It became a pyramid and took off.
5 nodes, part 1
G9: There was a tree
G10: Which became a big starship
G11: But the big steering wheel didn’t work
G12: So they took it to a warehouse (because it was big)
G13: Put it upside down and closed the bottom
G14: A man bent down to look at it
G15: Then called the pentagon
G16: Try a balloon, they said
G17: Or even better, a balloon with a vertical cut
G18: That’s more stable, said the man, looking more stable himself!
G19: Or even better, a balloon with a horizontal cut, which is parallel to the ground.
5 nodes, part 2
G20: Two guys liked the same three girls
G21: So they closed themselves in a sealed house of strange tables.
G22: Ordered a cone with two flavors of fried seafood
G23: They sat to eat it at a triangular table, one person sitting on the table giving food to three, but one was left out
G24: So they tried a counter with boys on one side and girls on the other, feeding with no intersections.
G25: The counter was too formal so they tried a rotated square table, with one person in the middle, but the person in the center could only serve one person at once.
G26: Meanwhile, the cone started dripping only on the side.
G27: So the person started rotating, serving everyone.
G28: By the centripetal force, the cone split into four flavors with dripping oil
G29: …which manifested divine forces of the Devil.
Carlos (notable emergent graphlet representations):
G2: Just K3.
G5: Just a square.
G8: Planar representation of K4.
G11: A cross.
G12: A house without a floor.
G13 (mentally coupled with G6): An esoteric symbol.
G15: Just a pentagon.
G16 (mentally coupled with G17 and G19): A lollipop.
G18 (mentally coupled with G14): An hourglass.
G21: A proper house.
G24: A bridge.
G29: Just K5.
Ethan (notable groupings and couplings)
G16, G17, and G19.
G12 and G21.
G12, G13, G14, G15, and G16.
G17, G18, and G19.
G20 and G22.
G26 and G28.
G25 and G27.
G28 and G29
graph theory, graphlets, mnemonics, phenomenology, math
For attribution, please cite this work as
Quintero (2023, July 21). Observations Regarding Graphlet Memorization. Retrieved from https://www.qri.org/blog/graphlet-memorization
BibTeX citation
@misc{quintero2023observations, author = {Quintero, Carlos Martinez}, title = {Observations Regarding Graphlet Memorization}, url = {https://www.qri.org/blog/graphlet-memorization}, year = {2023} }